Reconceptualizing cognitive media effects theory and research under the judged usability model
Keywords:
Media Effects, Cognitive Media Effects, Framing Effects, Agenda-setting, Priming News FramingAbstract
This review synthesizes the existing literature on cognitive media effects, including agenda setting, framing, and priming, in order to identify their similarities, differences, and inherent commonalities. Based on this review, we argue that the theory and research on each of these cognitive effects share a common view that media affect audience members by influencing the relative importance of considerations used to make subsequent judgments (including their answers to post-exposure survey questions). In reviewing this literature, we note that one important factor is often ignored, the extent to which a consideration featured in the message is deemed usable for a given subsequent judgment, a factor called judged usability, which may be an important mediator of cognitive media effects like agenda setting, framing and priming. Emphasizing judged usability leads to the revelation that media coverage may not just elevate a particular consideration, but may also actively suppress a consideration, rendering it less usable for subsequent judgments, opening a new avenue for cognitive effects research. In the interest of integrating these strands of cognitive effects research, we propose the Judged Usability Model as a revision of past cognitive models.
References
Althaus, S. L., & Kim, Y. M. (2006). Priming effects in complex information environments: Reassessing the impact of news discourse on presidential approval. The Journal of Politics, 68(4), 960-976. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2508.2006.00483.x
Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Anderson, N. H. (1981). Foundations of information integration theory. New York: Academic Press.
Cacciatore, M. A., Scheufele, D. A., & Iyengar, S. (2016). The end of framing as we know it… and the future of media effects. Mass Communication and Society, 19(1), 7-23. doi:10.1080/15205436.2015.1068811
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1984). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Advances in Consumer Research, 11(1), 673-675.
Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(5), 752-766. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.752
Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. P. Zanna, J. M. Olson, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario Symposium (Vol. 5, pp. 3-39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Chaiken, S., Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (1996). Principles of persuasion. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglansk (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. New York: Guilford Press.
Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007a). Framing theory. Annual Review of Political Science, 10, 103-126. doi:10.1146/annurev.polisci.10.072805.103054
Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007b). A theory of framing and opinion formation in competitive elite environments. Journal of Communication, 57(1), 99-118. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00331_2.x
Cohen, B. C. (1963). The press and foreign policy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407-428. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407
Corbett, J. B., & Durfee, J. L. (2004). Testing public (un)certainty of science media representations of global warming. Science Communication, 26(2), 129-151. doi:10.1177/1075547004270234
Croizet, J.-C., & Fiske, S. T. (2000). Moderation of priming by goals: Feeling entitled to judge increases judged usability of evaluative primes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36(2), 155-181. doi:10.1006/jesp.1999.1397
Danes, J. E., Hunter, J. E., & Woelfel, J. (1978). Mass communication and belief change: A test of three mathematical models. Human Communication Research, 4(3), 243-252. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1978.tb00613.x
Dearing, J. W. (1995). Newspaper coverage of maverick science: Creating controversy through balancing. Public Understanding of Science, 4(4), 341-361. doi:10.1088/0963-6625/4/4/002
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 5-18. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5
Druckman, J. N. (2001). The implications of framing effects for citizen competence. Political Behavior, 23(3), 225-256.
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.
Entman, R. M. (1993). Framing: Toward clarification of a fractured paradigm. Journal of Communication, 43(4), 51-58. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1993.tb01304.x
Entman, R. M. (2007). Framing bias: Media in the distribution of power. Journal of Communication, 57(1), 163-173. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00336.x
Entman, R. M. (2010). Media framing biases and political power: Explaining slant in news of Campaign 2008. Journalism, 11(4), 389-408. doi:10.1177/1464884910367587
Fazio, R. H., Blascovich, J., & Driscoll, D. M. (1992). On the functional value of attitudes: The influence of accessible attitudes on the ease and quality of decision making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18(4), 388-401. doi:10.1177/0146167292184002
Fischhoff, B., Slovic, P., & Lichtenstein, S. (1977). Knowing with certainty: The appropriateness of extreme confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3(4), 552-564. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.3.4.552
Fishbein, M. (1963). An investigation of the relationship between beliefs about an object and the attitude toward that object. Human Relations, 16(3), 233-239. doi:10.1177/001872676301600302
Geer, J. G. (1991). Do open-ended questions measure "salient" issues? Public Opinion Quarterly, 55(3), 360-370. doi:10.1086/269268
Gelbspan, R. (1998). The heat is on: The climate crisis, the cover-up, the prescription. New York: Perseus.
Hänze, M. (2001). Ambivalence, conflict, and decision making: Attitudes and feelings in Germany towards NATO's military intervention in the Kosovo war. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31(6), 693-706. doi:10.1002/ejsp.57
Higgins, E. T. (1989). Knowledge accessibility and activation: Subjectivity and suffering from unconscious sources. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought: The limits of awareness, intention and control (pp. 75-123). New York: Guilford.
Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles, 133–168.
Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1987). Social cognition and social perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 38(1), 369-425. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.38.020187.002101
Higgins, E. T., & Brendl, C. M. (1995). Accessibility and applicability: Some "activation rules" influencing judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31(3), 218-243. doi:10.1006/jesp.1995.1011
Higgins, E. T., Rholes, W. S., & Jones, C. R. (1977). Category accessibility and impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13(2), 141-154. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(77)80007-3
Iyengar, S. (1991). Is anyone responsible?: How television frames political issues. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Iyengar, S., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). News that matters. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Iyengar, S., Kinder, D. R., Peters, M. D., & Krosnick, J. A. (1984). The evening news and presidential evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(4), 778-787. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.46.4.778
Iyengar, S., & Simon, A. (1993). News coverage of the Gulf crisis and public opinion a study of agenda-setting, priming, and framing. Communication Research, 20(3), 365-383. doi:10.1177/009365093020003002
Jonas, K., Diehl, M., & Brömer, P. (1997). Effects of attitudinal ambivalence on information processing and attitude-intention consistency. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(2), 190-210. doi:10.1006/jesp.1996.1317
Kinder, D. R., & Sanders, L. M. (1990). Mimicking political debate with survey questions: The case of white opinion on affirmative action for blacks. Social Cognition, 8(1), 73-103. doi:10.1521/soco.1990.8.1.73
Kosicki, G. M. (1993). Problems and opportunities in agenda-setting research. Journal of Communication, 43(2), 100-127. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1993.tb01265.x
Krosnick, J. A., & Kinder, D. R. (1990). Altering the foundations of support for the president through priming. The American Political Science Review, 84(2), 497-512. doi:10.2307/1963531
Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: Cognitive and motivational bases: New York: Springer Science+Business Media.
Kruglanski, A. W., Dechesne, M., Orehek, E., & Pierro, A. (2009). Three decades of lay epistemics: The why, how, and who of knowledge formation. European Review of Social Psychology, 20(1), 146-191. doi:10.1080/10463280902860037
Krugman, P. (2015, January 11). For the love of carbon. The New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2015/01/12/opinion/paul-krugman-for-the-love-of-carbon.html?rref=collection%2Fcolumn%2Fpaul-krugman
Luce, M. F., Bettman, J. R., & Payne, J. W. (1997). Choice processing in emotionally difficult decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(2), 384-405. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.23.2.384
Maher, T. M. (2001). Framing: An emerging paradigm or a phase of agenda setting? In O. H. G. S. D. Reese, Jr., & A. E. Grant (Ed.), Framing public life: Perspectives on media and our understanding of the social world (pp. 83-94). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Martin, L. L. (1986). Set/reset: Use and disuse of concepts in impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(3), 493-504. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.493
Martin, L. L., & Achee, J. W. (1992). Beyond accessibility: The role of processing objects in judgment. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 195-216). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Matthes, J. (2006). The need for orientation towards news media: Revising and validating a classic concept. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 18(4), 422-444. doi:10.1093/ijpor/edh118
McCombs, M. E. (2004). Setting the agenda: The mass media and public opinion. Malden, MA: Polity.
McCombs, M. E., & Ghanem, S. I. (2001). The convergence of agenda setting and framing. In S. D. Reese, J. Oscar H. Gandy, & A. E. Grant (Eds.), Framing public life: Perspectives on media and our understanding of the social world (pp. 67-81). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.
McCombs, M. E., & Shaw, D. L. (1972). The agenda-setting function of mass media. Public Opinion Quarterly, 36(2), 176-187. doi:10.1086/267990
McCombs, M. E., Shaw, D. L., & Weaver, D. H. (1997). Communication and democracy: Exploring the intellectual frontiers in agenda-setting theory. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McGraw, K. M., & Ling, C. (2003). Media priming of presidential and group evaluations. Political Communication, 20(1), 23-40. doi:10.1080/10584600390172338
Miller, J. M. (2007). Examining the mediators of agenda setting: A new experimental paradigm reveals the role of emotions. Political Psychology, 28(6), 689-717. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2007.00600.x
Miller, J. M., & Krosnick, J. A. (2000). News media impact on the ingredients of presidential evaluations: Politically knowledgeable citizens are guided by a trusted source. American Journal of Political Science, 44(2), 301-315. doi:10.2307/2669312
Nelson, T. E., Clawson, R. A., & Oxley, Z. M. (1997). Media framing of a civil liberties conflict and its effect on tolerance. American Political Science Review, 91(3), 567-583. doi:10.2307/2952075
Nelson, T. E., & Oxley, Z. M. (1999). Issue framing effects on belief importance and opinion. The Journal of Politics, 61(4), 1040-1067. doi:10.2307/2647553
Nelson, T. E., Oxley, Z. M., & Clawson, R. A. (1997). Toward a psychology of framing effects. Political Behavior, 19(3), 221-246. doi:10.1023/A:1024834831093
Page, B. I., & Shapir, R. Y. (1992). The rational public. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Pan, Z., & Kosicki, G. M. (1997). Priming and media impact on the evaluations of the president's performance. Communication Research, 24(1), 3-30. doi:10.1177/009365097024001001
Petty, R., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2012). Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: Springer Science+Business Media.
Price, V., & Tewksbury, D. (1997). News values and public opinion: A theoretical account of media priming and framing. In G. Barnett & F. Boster (Eds.), Progress in communication sciences (Vol. 13, pp. 173-212). Greenwich, CT: Ablex.
Price, V., Tewksbury, D., & Powers, E. (1997). Switching trains of thought: The impact of news frames on readers' cognitive responses. Communication Research, 24(5), 481-506. doi:10.1177/009365097024005002
Scheufele, D. A. (2000). Agenda-setting, priming, and framing revisited: Another look at cognitive effects of political communication. Mass Communication and Society, 3(2), 297-316. doi:10.1207/S15327825MCS0323_07
Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1980). Public opinion and public ignorance: The fine line between attitudes and nonattitudes. American Journal of Sociology, 85(5) 1214-1225. doi:10.1086/227131
Shah, D. V., Watts, M. D., Domke, D., & Fan, D. P. (2002). News framing and cueing of issue regimes: Explaining Clinton's public approval in spite of scandal. Public Opinion Quarterly, 66(3), 339-370. doi:10.1086/341396
Shen, F. (2004). Chronic accessibility and individual cognitions: Examining the effects of message frames in political advertisements. Journal of Communication, 54(1), 123-137. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2004.tb02617.x
Sherif, M., & Hovland, C. I. (1961). Social judgment: Assimilation and contrast effects in communication and attitude change. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 973-988. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.973
Van der Pligt, J., & Eiser, J. R. (1984). Dimensional salience, judgment, and attitudes. In R. Eiser (Ed.), Attitudinal judgment (pp. 161-177). New York: Springer-Verlag.
West, D. M. (2005). Air wars: Television advertising in election campaigns, 1952-2004 (4th ed.). Washington, D.C.: CQ Press.
Zaller, J. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Downloads
Published
Issue
Section
License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.
The philosophy of the journal is to be open and to make all articles accessible. It is our belief that open access is a must in the future of science.
Authors who publish with RCR accept a slightly modified Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 3.0 Unported License.
You, as author, retain the copyrights for your paper, but the Review of Communication Research is granted exclusivity for publication of the article. The agreement allows others to share the work with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and publication in this journal. We do not want third parties to make a commercial use of the article, unless we agree it with authors.
The journal will run an open review process as well as a traditional peer review process.
When the manuscript is accepted for publication, it will get a doi number and get available online to facilitate early citation.
The journal will post the published article to many public repositories for further diffusion and permanence.
You, as author, are permitted and encouraged to post your work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on your website), as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work.
If you have any doubts, please, contact the editor: editor@rcommunication.org
Many thanks for submitting your work to this journal.